Regulatory focus and deliberate ignorance: When perceived accessibility leads to the decision to ignore information

Authors

  • Raymond Godwin Faculty of Psychology, Universitas Indonesia, Depok, 16424, Indonesia; Department of Psychology, Faculty of Humanities, Bina Nusantara University, Jakarta, 11480, Indonesia
  • Agnes Sianipar Faculty of Psychology, Universitas Indonesia, Depok, 16424, Indonesia
  • Bagus Takwin Faculty of Psychology, Universitas Indonesia, Depok, 16424, Indonesia

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.38140/ijspsy.v5i2.1951

Keywords:

Anticipated emotions, Deliberate ignorance, Perceived importance of knowing, Perceived accessibility, Regulatory focus

Abstract

While extensive research has focused on information seeking, the phenomenon of deliberate ignorance, where individuals choose not to know available information despite negligible acquisition costs and potentially high benefits, remains less understood. This study investigated the interplay of regulatory focus, perceived importance of learning, and perceived accessibility in predicting deliberate ignorance among young Indonesians. Employing a within-subjects, vignette-based experimental design with 114 respondents, we observed deliberate ignorance across all manipulated regulatory focus conditions. Generalised Estimating Equations revealed a significant interaction: a promotion focus, driven by the anticipation of pleasant emotions, strengthened the positive effect of perceived accessibility on deliberate ignorance. Conversely, the perceived importance of knowing consistently reduced deliberate ignorance, independent of regulatory focus. These findings highlight a nuanced relationship between cognitive factors, anticipated emotions-based regulatory focus, and the decision to remain ignorant. Consistent with regulatory focus theory, our results suggest that a promotion focus heightens sensitivity to potential emotional losses, increasing the likelihood of ignoring easily accessible information. Since most of the information offered in this research was internet-based, online communicators should be attentive to how information accessibility interacts with promotion-focused motivations. At the same time, educators can leverage these insights to empower young adults with strategies for navigating digital information overload.

References

Afifi, W. A., & Weiner, J. L. (2004). Toward a theory of motivated information management. Communication Theory, 14(2), 167–190. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.2004.tb00310.x

Barbour, J. B., Rintamaki, L. S., Ramsey, J. A., & Brashers, D. E. (2012). Avoiding health information. Journal of Health Communication, 17(2), 212–229. https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2011.585691

Baumgartner, H., Pieters, R., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2008). Future-oriented emotions: Conceptualization and behavioral effects. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38(4), 685–696. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.467

Bawden, D., & Robinson, L. (2009). The dark side of information: Overload, anxiety and other paradoxes and pathologies. Journal of Information Science, 35(2), 180–191. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165551508095781

Bechara, A., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). The somatic marker hypothesis: A neural theory of economic decision. Games and Economic Behavior, 52(2), 336–372. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.geb.2004.06.010

Case, D. O., Andrews, J. E., Johnson, J. D., & Allard, S. L. (2005). Avoiding versus seeking: The relationship of information seeking to avoidance, blunting, coping, dissonance, and related concepts. Journal of the Medical Library Association, 93(3), 353–362.

Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Connaway, L. S., Dickey, T. J., & Radford, M. L. (2011). “If it is too inconvenient I’m not going after it:” Convenience as a critical factor in information-seeking behaviors. Library and Information Science Research, 33(3), 179–190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lisr.2010.12.002

DeNicola, D. R. (2017). Understanding ignorance: The surprising impact of what we don’t know. MIT.

Ehrich, K. R., & Irwin, J. R. (2005). Willful ignorance in the request for product attribute information. Journal of Marketing Research, 42(3), 266–277. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.2005.42.3.266

Engel, C., & Hertwig, R. (2021). Deliberate ignorance: Present and future. In R. Hertwig & C. Engel (Eds.), Deliberate ignorance: Choosing not to know (pp. 333–348). MIT Press.

Fergusson, A. (2016). Designing online experiments using Google forms+ random redirect tool. https://teaching.statistics-is-awesome.org/designing-online-experiments-using-google-forms-random-redirect-tool

Frampton, J. R., & Fox, J. (2018). Social media’s role in romantic partners’ retroactive jealousy: Social comparison, uncertainty, and information seeking. Social Media and Society, 4(3), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1177/2056305118800317

Gigerenzer, G., & Garcia-Retamero, R. (2017). Cassandra’s regret: The psychology of not wanting to know. Psychological Review, 124(2), 179–196. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000055

Golman, R., & Loewenstein, G. (2018). Decision presence and absence of information and absence of information. Decision, 5(3), 143–164. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dec0000068

Golman, R., Hagmann, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2017). Information avoidance. Journal of Economic Literature, 55(1), 96–135. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2633226

Grimani, A., Yemiscigil, A., Wang, Q., Kirilov, G., Kudrna, L., & Vlaev, I. (2024). How do emotions respond to outcome values and influence choice? Psychological Research, 88(8), 2234–2250. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-024-02001-3

Grossman, Z., & van der Weele, J. J. (2017). Self-image and willful ignorance in social decisions. Journal of the European Economic Association, 15(1), 173–217. https://doi.org/10.1093/jeea/jvw001

Hertwig, R. (2012). The psychology and rationality of decisions from experience. In Synthese (Vol. 187, Issue 1, pp. 269–292). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-011-0024-4

Hertwig, R., & Engel, C. (2016). Homo ignorans: Deliberately choosing not to know. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(3), 359–372. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616635594

Hertwig, R., Woike, J. K., & Schupp, J. (2021). Age differences in deliberate ignorance. Psychology and Aging, 36(4), 407–414. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000603

Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52(12), 1280–1300. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.52.12.1280

Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fit. American Psychologist, 55(11), 1217–1230. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.11.1217

Ho, E. H., Hagmann, D., & Loewenstein, G. F. (2020). Measuring information preferences. Management Science, 67(1), 126–145. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2019.3543

Howell, J. L., & Shepperd, J. A. (2016). Establishing an information avoidance scale. Psychological Assessment. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000315

Hussain, M., Price, D. M., Gesselman, A. N., Shepperd, J. A., & Howell, J. L. (2021). Avoiding information about one’s romantic partner. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 38(2), 626–647. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407520969856

Lancaster, A. L., Dillow, M. R., Ball, H., Borchert, K., & Tyler, W. J. C. (2016). Managing information about a romantic partner’s relationship history: An application of the Theory of Motivated Information Management. Southern Communication Journal, 81(2), 63–78. https://doi.org/10.1080/1041794X.2015.1089926

Lerner, J. S., Li, Y., Valdesolo, P., & Kassam, K. S. (2015). Emotion and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 799–823. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115043

Li, J. (2023). Information avoidance in the age of COVID-19: A meta-analysis. Information Processing and Management, 60(1), 103163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ipm.2022.103163

Mellers, B. A., & McGraw, A. P. (2001). Anticipated emotions as guides to choice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10(6), 210–214. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00151

Mellers, B. A., Schwartz, A., & Ritov, I. (1999). Emotion-based choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(3), 332–345. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.128.3.332

Misra, S., & Stokols, D. (2012). Psychological and health outcomes of perceived information overload. Environment and Behavior, 44(6), 737–759. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916511404408

Moradi, H., & Nesterov, A. (2017). Moral wiggle room reverted: Information avoidance is myopic. In Basic Research Program (WP BRP 189/EC/2018; Economics). https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3168630

Oosterwijk, S. (2017). Choosing the negative: A behavioral demonstration of morbid curiosity. PLoS ONE, 12(7), e0178399. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0178399

Robson, A., & Robinson, L. (2013). Building on models of information behaviour: Linking information seeking and communication. Journal of Documentation, 69(2), 169–193. https://doi.org/10.1108/00220411311300039

Schwartz, B., Richerson, P. J., Berkman, B. E., Frankenreiter, J., Hagmann, D., Isaacowitz, D. M., Pachur, T., Schooler, L. J., & Wehling, P. (2021). The deep structure of deliberate ignorance: Mapping the terrain. In R. Hertwig & C. Engel (eds.), Deliberate ignorance: Choosing not to know (pp. 65–88). MIT Press.

Shepperd, J. A., & Howell, J. L. (2015). Responding to psychological threats with deliberate ignorance: Causes and remedies. Handbook of Personal Security, 10994, 257–274. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315713595.CH16

Shipp, T. D., Shipp, D. Z., Bromley, B., Sheahan, R., Cohen, A., Lieberman, E., & Benacerraf, B. (2004). What factors are associated with parents’ desire to know the sex of their unborn child? Birth, 31(4), 272–279. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0730-7659.2004.00319.x

Simon, H. A. (1993). Decision making: Rational, nonrational, and irrational. Educational Administration Quarterly, 29(3), 392–411. https://www.learntechlib.org/p/145912/

Soroya, S. H., Farooq, A., Mahmood, K., Isoaho, J., & Zara, S. E. (2021). From information seeking to information avoidance: Understanding the health information behavior during a global health crisis. Information Processing and Management, 58(2), 102440. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ipm.2020.102440

Spitzer, M. W. H., Janz, J., Nie, M., & Kiesel, A. (2024). On the interplay of curiosity, confidence, and importance in knowing information. Psychological Research, 88(1), 101–115. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-023-01841-9

Sweeny, K., Melnyk, D., Miller, W., & Shepperd, J. A. (2010). Information avoidance: Who, what, when, and why. Review of General Psychology, 14(4), 340–353. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021288

Taber, J. M., Klein, W. M. P., Ferrer, R. A., Lewis, K. L., Harris, P. R., Shepperd, J. A., & Biesecker, L. G. (2015). Information avoidance tendencies, threat management resources, and interest in genetic sequencing feedback. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 49(4), 616–621. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-014-9679-7

van Dijk, E., & Zeelenberg, M. (2007). When curiosity killed regret: Avoiding or seeking the unknown in decision-making under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(4), 656–662. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.06.004

Published

2025-06-30

How to Cite

Godwin, R., Sianipar, A., & Takwin, B. (2025). Regulatory focus and deliberate ignorance: When perceived accessibility leads to the decision to ignore information. International Journal of Studies in Psychology, 5(2), 46-54. https://doi.org/10.38140/ijspsy.v5i2.1951